banner
Home / News / The prevalence of chronic and episodic loneliness and social isolation from a longitudinal survey
News

The prevalence of chronic and episodic loneliness and social isolation from a longitudinal survey

Aug 02, 2023Aug 02, 2023

Scientific Reports volume 13, Article number: 12453 (2023) Cite this article

Loneliness and social isolation, experienced more long-term, has been shown to increase mortality and lead to poorer health outcomes in specific cohorts. However, it is unclear what the prevalence of chronic loneliness and social isolation is, and which demographic groups are most at risk of reporting more chronic forms. A psychometrically validated classification system was used to identify people who met criteria for episodic and chronic loneliness and social isolation using the Household Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey waves 14–18. The prevalence of loneliness (overall 34%; 21% episodic, 13% chronic) far exceeded that of social isolation (overall 17%; 13% episodic, 4% chronic). There was consistency in the demographic characteristics (from age, sex, household type, income) of those who experienced loneliness and social isolation. However, people with a long-term health condition had an elevated risk of episodic loneliness (AOR 1.24, 95% CI 1.11–1.39) and a markedly higher risk of chronic loneliness (AOR 2.01, 95% CI 1.76–2.29), compared with those without a long-term health condition. Loneliness, both episodic and chronic subtypes, is more prevalent than social isolation. However, both chronic loneliness and social isolation remains neglected and poorly targeted within current practice and policy.

Being socially connected is fundamental to human health and wellbeing1. For decades, social isolation, characterised by an objective lack of social contact and connections2,3, has been widely acknowledged as a risk factor for broad-based mortality and morbidity4,5. But there has been a growing number of studies that have found similar harmful effects of loneliness on health6. Loneliness—sometimes described as ‘perceived social isolation’, is characterised by distressing feelings when one perceives a discrepancy between desired and actual relationships7. Loneliness is regarded as a biopsychosocial stressor8,9, and associated with a multitude of poor health outcomes, from cardiometabolic disease, stroke10, dementia11, and depression12. It is therefore not just having social connections but also reporting feeling socially connected to others, that holds implications for one’s risk of morbidity and mortality13,14.

Both social isolation and loneliness share similar associations with demographic, socio-economic and health factors5,15. Socio-demographic characteristics such as age, gender, living alone, socioeconomic status, migrant status and employment are all factors that have been examined in both the social isolation and loneliness research3. Older adults are often perceived to be more vulnerable to loneliness and social isolation as compared with other age groups16. However, it is now widely accepted that loneliness and social isolation affects everyone across the life course17.

The impacts of social isolation and loneliness on health are further influenced by a broader number of factors including the social determinants of health in which people may be born into or live under18. For example, in studies on social isolation, factors including neighbourhood disadvantage and migrant status play a role in moderating poorer health outcomes including myocardial infarction risk19, colorectal cancer risk20, carotid stiffness21, and psychological wellbeing22. Current models of loneliness acknowledge the influence of the social environment and resources available to the lonely individual15,23,24, and it has been reported that some individuals are more likely to break out of the cycle of loneliness than others25. Indeed, poor health, be it physical or mental26, influences one’s capacity to participate socially in their networks including friends, colleagues, and the wider community. Both loneliness and social isolation are linked to higher incidence of chronic disease27,28.

Population surveys show varying prevalence rates of loneliness and social isolation across different samples and stages. The reported global prevalence rate in a recent meta-analysis noted that the prevalence of loneliness in adolescents (12–17 years old) differed depending on the region (9.2% South-East Asian to 14.4% Eastern Mediterranean countries) and for young people (18–29 years old) was 5.3%29. Middle aged adults (30–59 years old) reported a pooled prevalence estimate of 6.9% across different parts of European, and for older adults (over 60 years) reported anywhere from 5.2% (Northern Europe), 8.7% (Western Europe), 15.7% Southern European to the highest 21.3% for Eastern Europeans countries29.

While there is robust scientific literature on the negative impact of loneliness and social isolation on health, there has been far less scrutiny on the prevalence rates, sociodemographic risk factors, and the health impacts of persistent experiences (i.e., chronic) loneliness and social isolation. Specifically, social isolation is well-known to be detrimental for health and wellbeing4,5 but the evidence on chronic social isolation is largely informed by numerous animal studies (due to ethical reasons)6 or retrospective epidemiological data30. The study of chronic loneliness on the other hand, remains underexamined because most of this work has been reliant on cross-sectional study designs which may not capture the persistence or stability of these experiences31. Many loneliness measures, both direct, such as Office of National Statistics Loneliness item32, and indirect measures UCLA-Loneliness Scale33 and De Jong Gierveld Social and Emotional Loneliness scale34 often capture loneliness through the frequency of lonely feelings (i.e., how often do you feel isolated from others?). This is different to the capturing the persistence of lonely feelings (i.e., how long have you felt isolated from others?) or intensity (e.g., how distressed did you feel?). Attention to the cumulative impact of more chronic experiences of loneliness on poorer health outcomes is growing35 and examined within particular cohorts including older adults36,37,38 and adolescents39. For instance, chronic loneliness has been found to be associated with an 80% greater risk of death in Americans aged 50 and older (situational HR 1.56%; 95% CI 1.52–1.62 versus chronic HR 1.83, 95% CI 1.71–1.87)37.

This study will add to the growing literature on how chronic forms of loneliness and social isolation affects different demographic groups. First, we aimed to identify the proportion of people who experience loneliness and social isolation persistently over time (i.e., chronicity), as opposed to transient periods (i.e., episodic). Second, we aimed to identify socio-demographic characteristics that were associated with episodic or chronic loneliness and social isolation.

Data used in this study were collected from the Household Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey a household-based panel study of over 9,000 Australian households conducted annually, with wave 1 starting in 200140. A number of household-level and person-level questionnaires were completed for each household. This study uses data from the self-completion questionnaire, which is undertaken by individuals aged 15 years or older in each participating household, from waves 14–18 (5 years). We included cases that did not have missing data for loneliness and social isolation items across all waves. In total, we had 10,746 participants from waves 14–18 with no missing loneliness items, and 10,918 participants with no missing social isolation items from waves 14–18.

The HILDA survey collects data on household and family relationships, income, education, employment, living situation, lifestyle behaviours and general health and wellbeing.

Previous estimates of the prevalence of loneliness among HILDA participants have been based on responses to the single-item statement “I often feel very lonely”41. Using a single item may however lead to underreporting due to the known stigma of loneliness42. To assess loneliness and social isolation, we used the scales derived from 10-items Index of Social Support scale43 in the HILDA questionnaire (see Supplementary Fig. 1 in psychometric validation paper)44. The loneliness scale included three items: “People don’t come to visit me as often as I would like”, “I often need help from other people but can’t get it” and “I often feel very lonely”. The social isolation scale included four items: “There is someone who can always cheer me up when I’m down”, “I enjoy the time I spend with the people who are important to me”, “When something’s on my mind, just talking with the people I know can make me feel better” and “When I need someone to help me out, I can usually find someone”. Psychometric analysis has shown that each of these scales has good internal reliability and construct validity44. We classified people who were lonely to have a median score of greater than 4, and for social isolation a median item score of less than 4, on the scales developed for each of these conditions (see methodology in psychometric validation paper)44.

To date, the study of the impact of chronic loneliness and social isolation is in its infancy. In this study, we defined episodic loneliness and social isolation as meeting criteria for any episode or number of episodes that were not consecutive (e.g., only wave 15, or both waves 15 and 17). Chronic loneliness and social isolation were defined as meeting criteria for two or more consecutive episodes (e.g., waves 14 and 15, or waves 16, 17, and 18); in other words, a minimum of two consecutive years. Episodic and chronic loneliness, and episodic and chronic social isolation were mutually exclusive, and each person was categorised as have episodic, chronic, or no loneliness, and episodic, chronic, or no social isolation across the five-year period (waves 14–18).

The term ‘chronic’ is therefore used to indicate that the experience is prolonged or persistent over a longer period of time and may be resistant to change. This is consistent with American Psychological Association’s definition of chronicity (see https://dictionary.apa.org/chronic). While loneliness and social isolation are social experiences and not diseases, our classification of what is chronic is also aligned to Center for Disease Control’s definition of chronic disease45.

We identified nine socio-demographic characteristics known to be associated with loneliness, social isolation, or both based on previous research.

For sex only two categories were used: identifying as male or female. Age was categorised into five groups: 15–29 years; 30–44 years; 45–59 years; 60–74 years; or 75 years and over. Loneliness and social isolation are known to vary across the lifespan, and this can also vary across sex17,31,46, hence examining the impact of loneliness and social isolation across both sex and age is critical.

Two socio-demographic characteristics that could influence the degree of social interaction one may have include household structure (i.e., living status) and employment. Living alone and unemployment have been shown to contribute to loneliness47,48,49 and social isolation50 in different populations. Household structure was categorised as follows: (1) couple with child; (2) couple without child; (3) lone parent with child; (4) lone person; or (5) other, which included unrelated household members, other family members, and single or double-parent households with a child over the age of 15. The employment status of participants was classified as: (1) full-time; (2) part-time; (3) retired; (4) unemployed; or 5) other (which included home duties, non-working students and unspecified).

An established social identity51 and ability to communicate in the primary language in the country one lives in52 were identified as socio-demographic characteristics that could influence social integration. Indeed, being of ethnic minority status is increasingly being examined as an increased risk for loneliness49 and social isolation53. Because migrant status was not directly available, we selected country of birth and language spoke at home. Country of birth was categorised as: (1) Australia; (2) Other English-speaking country; or (3) Other non-English speaking country. Language spoken at home was categorised as (1) English or (2) other.

Two socio-demographic characteristics were selected to indicate economic status, which were household income and neighbourhood disadvantage, both well known to influence loneliness and social isolation. More generally, lower household income and increased neighbourhood disadvantage is associated with increased risk for loneliness and social isolation48,54. Household income was categorised as three levels: (1) less than $80,000; (2) $80,000–$149,999; or (3) more than $150,000. In order to determine relative neighbourhood disadvantage, the composite Socio-Economic Index for Areas and Index of Relative Socio-Economic Advantage and Disadvantage (SEIFA IRSAD) was used. This captures several variables including income, education, employment, occupation, and housing characteristics at the postcode level, based on the Australian Population Census55. Participants were classified into a SEIFA IRSAD quintile, based on their postcode of residence, with quintile 5 indicating the highest levels of socio-economic advantage and quintile 1 the lowest levels of socio-economic advantage.

We also included a measure of the presence of long-term health conditions (i.e., chronic disease) to examine how this relates to both loneliness and social isolation. In large epidemiological studies, loneliness and social isolation have been found to be associated with a higher incidence of chronic disease10,28,56. This categorical variable (Yes/No) was coded based on self-reporting of at least one of many listed physical health conditions (e.g., heart disease, arthritis, chronic pain, hearing, or speech problems).

In order to derive prevalence estimates of loneliness and social isolation, the longitudinal weights provided by HILDA were used, which adjust for attrition and ensure that the sample is representative of the Australian population distribution of age, sex, state, labour force status, marital status, and household composition. A detailed description of the HILDA survey weights is available elsewhere57. First, we estimated the prevalence of episodic, chronic or any loneliness and social isolation across socio-demographic characteristics and across the HILDA waves 14 (2014) to 18 (2018). Second, we conducted univariable and multivariable logistic regression to assess the difference in episodic, chronic, or either subtype (episodic or chronic) of loneliness and isolation based on the specified socio-demographic characteristics. See Table 1 for episodic and chronic loneliness and Table 2 for episodic and chronic social isolation.

No ethics approval and consent were required for this study. This project was granted exemption from ethics review from The University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee.

At least 34% of participants reported either episodic or chronic loneliness during waves 14 (2014) to 18 (2018). Of these, 21% experienced an episode of loneliness and 13% experienced chronic loneliness. The prevalence of social isolation was lower than that of loneliness. Only 17% of participants reported either episodic or chronic social isolation during waves 14 (2014) to 18 (2018). Of this, 13% reported episodic social isolation, and 4% reported chronic social isolation (See Fig. 1).

Prevalence of (A) episodic loneliness, (B) chronic loneliness, (C) no loneliness; (D) episodic social isolation, (E) chronic social isolation, and (F) no social isolation across 5 years.

Findings from the analysis of the relationship between socio-demographic and health factors and the dependent variables of loneliness and social isolation are shown in Tables 1 and 2, respectively.

Being male was associated with lower likelihood of reporting loneliness (episodic or chronic) compared to females by 11% (OR 0.89, 95% CI 0.82–0.96), but these relationships became insignificant once adjusted for all covariates. Conversely, being male was associated with a higher likelihood of both episodic (AOR 1.23, 95% CI 1.09–1.38) and chronic social isolation (AOR 1.78; 95%CI 1.45–2.20).

In order to examine differences across age groups, we used 30–44 years as the reference category given that previous studies have reported this group as being less lonely than the younger age group (15–29 years) and we wished to determine the magnitude of any increased risk in the youngest group. When compared to those aged 30–44 years, younger people aged 15–29 were less likely to report chronic loneliness (AOR 0.77, 95% CI 0.62–0.94). However, those aged 60–74 years and those 75 + also showed the same trend reporting significantly less chronic loneliness than those aged 30–44 years. (60–74: AOR 0.62, 95% CI 0.49–0.78; 75 +: AOR 0.59 95% CI 0.43–0.81). Those aged 45–49 were also less likely to experience either subtype of loneliness (AOR 0.85, 95% CI 0.75–0.97). In regard to social isolation, only the 45–59 age group reported significantly more of either subtype of social isolation than those aged 30–44 years (AOR 1.19 95% CI 1.01–1.40).

When compared with couples with children, living alone (AOR 1.66, 95% CI 1.36–2.03) and being a lone parent with a child increased the likelihood of chronic loneliness (AOR 1.64, 95% CI 1.19–2.27). Couples without children were significantly less likely to experience chronic loneliness (AOR 0.75, 95% CI 0.62–0.92) when compared with couples with children. Similarly, when compared with couples with children, people who lived alone or in other household types were more likely to be episodically or chronically social isolated. Lone parents with children were more likely to be episodically socially isolated compared to couples with children (AOR 1.50, 95% CI 1.05–2.15).

Those born in a country other than Australia (non-English speaking) are more likely to report episodic loneliness than those born in Australia (AOR 1.22, 95% CI 1.01–1.48), but were significantly less likely to report chronic loneliness (AOR 0.83, 95% CI 0.69–1.00). There were no differences in levels of loneliness between people who spoke a language other than English and people who spoke English. People born outside of Australia both in English and non-English speaking countries did not report an increased likelihood of social isolation compared with those born in Australia. Compared with people who spoke only English, those who spoke a language other than English did not show higher levels of social isolation after results were fully adjusted (all ps > 0.05).

Compared to participants who reported the highest income (over $150,000), participants who reported the lowest income (< $80,000) and those who reported 80,000–149,000 had an increased likelihood of both episodic (< $80,000: AOR 1.49, 95% CI 1.27–1.74; $80,000–149,000: AOR 1.27 95% CI 1.10–1.45) and chronic loneliness (< $80,000: AOR 1.66, 95% CI 1.36–2.02; $80,000–149,000: AOR 1.26 95% CI 1.05–1.50). In addition, participants who reported the lowest income (< $80,000) had an increased likelihood of both episodic (AOR 1.48, 95% CI 1.22–1.80) and chronic social isolation (AOR 1.83, 95% CI 1.30–2.57). Those who those earned between $80,0000 to $149,999, showed an increased likelihood of episodic social isolation (AOR 1.23, 95% CI 1.04–1.47) when compared to those who earned over $150,000, but not chronic social isolation.

When compared with those in full-time employment, those who were unemployed and those who in ‘other’ category of employment (i.e., home duties, non-working students) were more likely to report episodic (unemployed: AOR 1.33, 95% CI 1.01–1.77; other: AOR 1.21, 95% CI 1.02–1.77) and chronic loneliness (unemployed: AOR 1.66, 95% CI 1.22–2.25; other: AOR 1.57, 95% CI 1.29–1.91). Those who were unemployed and those who in ‘other’ category were also more likely than those in full-time employment to report episodic (unemployed: AOR 1.43, 95% CI 1.03–1.97; other: AOR 1.47, 95% CI 1.19–1.80) and chronic social isolation (unemployed: AOR 2.46, 95% CI 1.88–3.24; other: AOR 2.21, 95% CI 1.88–2.60).

Compared with participants in SEIFA IRSAD 5 (most advantaged), only participants in SEIFA IRSAD quintiles 1 (least advantaged) showed an increased likelihood of both episodic (AOR 1.36 95% CI 1.16–1.59) and chronic loneliness (AOR 1.34 95% CI 1.11–1.62). On the other hand, participants in SEIFA IRSAD quintiles 1 to 2 had an increased likelihood of both episodic social isolation (quintile 1: AOR 1.63, 95% CI 1.35–1.97; quintile 2: AOR 1.24, 95% CI 1.03–1.50) and chronic social isolation (quintile 1: AOR 1.69, 95% CI 1.15–2.48, quintile 2: AOR 1.77, 95% CI 1.21–2.59). Those in quintiles 3 and 4 had an increased likelihood of chronic social isolation (quintile 3: AOR 2.33, 95% CI 1.61–3.38; quintile 4: AOR 2.11, 95% CI 1.46–3.06) compared with those in quintile 5.

Participants who reported a long-term health condition showed an increased likelihood of both episodic (AOR 1.24, 95% CI 1.11–1.39) and chronic loneliness (AOR 2.01, 95% CI 1.76–2.29) than those who did not. In addition, participants who reported a long-term health condition reported an increased likelihood of both episodic (AOR 1.34, 95% CI 1.17–1.52), and chronic social isolation (AOR 1.87, 95% CI 1.50–2.34).

Loneliness and social isolation have been recognised as important public health issues in many countries. While these conditions have similar negative health impacts, it is unclear who is more likely to experience chronic loneliness and social isolation. This study is the first to distinguish episodic from chronic experiences of loneliness and social isolation with the use of longitudinal population data, and to identify who is most vulnerable.

It was noteworthy that the cumulative prevalence rates of loneliness (overall 34%; 21% episodic, 13% chronic) far exceeded the prevalence rates of social isolation (overall 17%; 13% episodic, 4% chronic) in Australia. It is also possible that the cumulative prevalence rate of loneliness in this study is underestimated, given that the HILDA participants themselves may be more engaged or less socially isolated than the general Australian community. However, more programs and policies have focussed on reducing social isolation and fewer on reducing loneliness—perhaps simply because it is easier to measure a reduction in social isolation (i.e., objective, quantifiable, and or observable) as opposed to the challenges of measuring loneliness (i.e., subjective, qualitative). For example, employing strategies that increase social contact and opportunities (i.e., reduce social isolation) may not also lead to the development and maintenance of meaningful connections, which could reduce loneliness. Several factors may be driving this, including a gap in research translation to practice (e.g., community and health practitioners not measuring loneliness severity) to poor community awareness about what loneliness is (e.g., confusion with social isolation or stigma associated with loneliness)58.

Being male was protective of any type (episodic or chronic) of loneliness, consistent with previous study which report women are more predisposed to loneliness59, including older women60. Men, however, were more likely to be socially isolated than women, which is consistent with sex differences in reporting subjective versus objective social isolation61,62. Although men were more socially isolated, they may be either less vulnerable or more reluctant to report loneliness.

Our results indicate that age differences were only present in those that reported chronic, as opposed to episodic, loneliness. Much of the research to date does not make the distinction between these sub-types, but most point to a U-shaped distribution where younger and older people are more vulnerable to problematic levels of loneliness16,49. After accounting for all possible confounding variables, those aged 75 years and older, followed by those aged 60–74 years, and those 15–29 years were all less likely to experience chronic loneliness than those aged 30–44 years. One plausible reason for the greater vulnerability of individuals aged 30–44 years could include a lack of time to nurture and maintain meaningful social relationships.

Interestingly, the only age-related difference in social isolation was the higher prevalence among those aged 45–59 years of either subtype. While there is a strong focus upon social isolation in older adults in both research and public policy, our findings indicate there is a need to assist individuals in the middle-aged, pre-retirement phase of life. Those aged 45–59 years were also less likely to experience any episodic or chronic loneliness, which demonstrates that these constructs are likely to be independent.

Overall, these findings challenge previous studies about who is lonely and who is socially isolated. These differing results are likely to be due to the heterogeneity of samples and different measures of loneliness and social isolation used, and the inclusion of confounding factors (e.g. household structure) are often not included in other studies. Further, our psychometrically derived scales for loneliness and social isolation may be robust but is only specifically derived from the HILDA44. This makes it difficult to directly compare with other studies and more research is needed to understand the specific factors that may drive social vulnerability in particular groups.

Our findings indicate that single parents with young children have an elevated risk of chronic loneliness, almost equivalent to those who live alone. Similarly, an earlier study using the HILDA sample also indicate that single fathers with children were at risk of loneliness43. While there no known studies examining the impact of loneliness in single parents, the detrimental impact is consistent with reports of poor health status63 and increased mortality risk in this population group64. It was noteworthy that couples without children were less likely to experience chronic loneliness when compared with couples with children. Further, compared with those without children, almost all groups reported higher levels of episodic social isolation but only those who lived alone and who lived with non-family members were more likely to experience chronic social isolation.

We found no differences between by country of birth (Australia, other English speaking, or non-English speaking) for episodic or chronic social isolation. On the other hand, people from non-English speaking countries were at more risk of episodic loneliness, while tending to have lower risk of chronic loneliness. This may signal that individuals from non-English speaking backgrounds are able to build meaningful social relationships as their duration of residence lengthens. Further research needs to be done comparing loneliness and social isolation between specific cultural groups with appropriate assessment tools, especially in light of research showing that loneliness is a significant predictor of lower self-reported health, greater risk of posttraumatic stress, and higher incidence of mental illness in migrant groups65.

Household income was found to have an inverse relationship with the risk of both episodic and chronic loneliness, that is, the lower the income the higher likelihood of reporting each sub-type of loneliness. Similar patterns were found for episodic social isolation, but only those who had the lowest income (< $80,000) reported higher likelihood of chronic social isolation. Plausible reasons for these trends include having fewer resources (i.e., time or money) to invest in developing and maintaining meaningful social connection (i.e., reducing their risk of loneliness and social isolation)61. It was also found that those who were unemployed showed the highest risk of both episodic and chronic loneliness, but compared with those in full-time employment, people engaged in home duties and non-working students showed the highest risk of episodic and chronic social isolation. This highlights the opportunities that employment can offer for building and developing social connection.

Consistent with previous research66,67,68, those living in more disadvantaged neighbourhoods reported more loneliness and social isolation compared with people living in more advantaged neighbourhoods. More advantaged neighbourhoods may offer more physical spaces and environmental resources (such as green spaces) that can be conducive to promoting social connection69. A similar clear trend was seen for episodic social isolation, with those in the most more disadvantaged neighbourhoods (SEIFA IRSAD 1–2) having the highest risk of episodic social isolation, whereas all groups (SEIFA IRSAD 1–4) were at greater risk of chronic social isolation than those on the most advantaged quintile (SEIFA IRSAD 5).

People who had a long-term health condition were consistently more likely to experience both loneliness and socially isolation, with this sub-group reporting approximately double the risk of chronic loneliness and isolation compared to those without a long-term health condition. Approximately, one in four (24%) individuals with a long-term health condition reported episodic loneliness and one in five (20%) met the criteria for chronic loneliness. While there is a plethora of research on how individual health characteristics (i.e., physical health, BMI)70,71, and poor health regulation behaviours (i.e., smoking, alcohol use, physical activity)72,73 influence loneliness and social isolation in specific demographic cohorts, there is a lack of clarity on how these factors influence the onset of chronic loneliness. Our findings nevertheless highlight the importance of preventing loneliness in these vulnerable groups, and the need to equip health care practitioners and community agencies to better support people with long-term health conditions to manage their psychosocial well-being74. This may be in the form of building linkages with others who have a shared experience and to facilitate greater participation with their existing social networks.

While the sample used in analysis was weighted to match the profile of the Australian population, it is possible that individuals taking part in the HILDA longitudinal study are unrepresentative in selected characteristics (social and psychological) that are of importance to this study and cannot be mitigated by means of demographic weighting. Further, these issues may be magnified by the fact participants under 18 were primarily recruited via their parents, who are themselves HILDA participants and this recruitment method may skew our results on loneliness and social isolation reported by young people under 18.

This study enabled examination of how loneliness and social isolation of differing levels of duration affect particular population groups, using psychometrically validated scales but the categorical classification did not enable investigation of the severity (i.e., intensity) of these experiences. The differences between episodic and chronicity of these social experiences is at its infancy. One outstanding question is whether the negative impact of an intense but short episodic of loneliness and social isolation is equivalent to the negative impact of a low intensity but persistent experience of loneliness and social isolation. Additionally, this study does not show the factors predicting a transition from episodic loneliness and social isolation to the chronic forms of these conditions, which remains an evidence-gap75.

This study did not examine more closely differences in how loneliness and social isolation influence different chronic health conditions76, as there is evidence that the pathway to disease could differ for loneliness and social isolation (e.g., loneliness better predicted poorer mental health; social isolation better predicted poorer physical and cognitive health)77, Previous relationships have been established between specific health conditions and loneliness and social isolation, for example, cardiovascular disease and Type 2 diabetes were associated with loneliness and social isolation, but the same effect was not found for other disorders including chronic obstructive pulmonary disease and cancer28.

This study provides an indication of who is vulnerable to loneliness and social isolation, especially within the Australian context. Our data shows clearly that economic and social factors influence our social connection—for example, people with low income, who are unemployed and live in disadvantaged neighbourhoods are at greater risk of loneliness and social isolation. Hence, these conditions are inherently linked with fundamental social determinants of health15.

The outstanding question is how can we effectively address loneliness and social isolation, and improve social connection in the long term? Many solutions adopt a downstream approach, to help people who are already lonely to manage their distressing feelings. This may involve delivering individually based (e.g., therapy) or community-based programs (e.g., improving neighbourhood connections). However, these solutions may offer only short term benefits if we do not consider a whole-of-systems approach to loneliness and social isolation.

There must be continued efforts of leveraging cross-sector collaborations (e.g., health, business, and community sectors) to also take upstream solutions (e.g., improving access to affordable housing, employment support). These upstream solutions offer high potential for preventing the onset of loneliness and social isolation and yield long-term benefits78.

Having social connection, be it having more contact with others or feeling meaningfully connected to them, is fundamental for health and wellbeing. Our findings indicate that loneliness when compared with social isolation both in episodic and chronic subtypes, is more prevalent than social isolation. Both chronic forms loneliness and social isolation remains neglected and poorly targeted within current practice and policy. Further, we have found that individuals who are socially disadvantaged and those with long-term health conditions are more likely to report episodic and chronic loneliness and social isolation. This highlights the need to better understand the psychological, economic, and environmental mechanisms that are contributing to loneliness and social isolation in these population groups, and to use this knowledge to develop policies and programs that address these critical dimensions of health and quality of life.

The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article are available to researchers living in Australia or overseas through the National Centre for Longitudinal Data Dataverse. Information about applying for access to the data is available at: https://dataverse.ada.edu.au/dataverse/ncld. This study was exempted from ethics review from The University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee.

Cacioppo, J. T. & Cacioppo, S. Social relationships and health: The toxic effects of perceived social isolation. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 8, 58–72. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12087 (2014).

Article Google Scholar

Coyle, C. E. & Dugan, E. Social isolation, loneliness and health among older adults. J. Aging Health 24, 1346–1363 (2012).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Victor, C., Scambler, S., Bond, J. & Bowling, A. Being alone in later life: Loneliness, social isolation and living alone. Rev. Clin. Geront. 10, 407–417 (2000).

Article Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., Shankar, A., Demakakos, P. & Wardle, J. Social isolation, loneliness, and all-cause mortality in older men and women. PNAS 110, 5797–5801. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1219686110 (2013).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Pantell, M. et al. Social isolation: A predictor of mortality comparable to traditional clinical risk factors. Am. J. Public Health 103, 2056–2062. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.2013.301261 (2013).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J. P. & Cole, S. W. The neuroendocrinology of social isolation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 733–767. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015240 (2015).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Peplau, L. & Perlman, D. Loneliness: A Sourcebook of Current Theory, Research and Therapy 1–20 (Wiley, 1982).

Google Scholar

Petitte, T. et al. A systematic review of loneliness and common chronic physical conditions in adults. Open Psychol. J. 8, 113–132. https://doi.org/10.2174/1874350101508010113 (2015).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J. P. & Cacioppo, J. T. Toward a neurology of loneliness. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1464–1504. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037618 (2014).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Valtorta, N. K., Kanaan, M., Gilbody, S., Ronzi, S. & Hanratty, B. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for coronary heart disease and stroke: Systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal observational studies. Heart 102, 1009–1016. https://doi.org/10.1136/heartjnl-2015-308790 (2016).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Kuiper, J. S. et al. Social relationships and risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Res. Rev. 22, 39–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2015.04.006 (2015).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Lim, M. H., Rodebaugh, T. L., Zyphur, M. J. & Gleeson, J. F. Loneliness over time: The crucial role of social anxiety. J Ab Psychol https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000162 (2016).

Article Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T. & Stephenson, D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: A meta-analytic review. Pers. Psychol. Sci. 10, 227–237. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614568352 (2015).

Article Google Scholar

Henriksen, J., Larsen, E. R., Mattisson, C. & Andersson, N. W. Loneliness, health and mortality. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 28, 234–239. https://doi.org/10.1017/s2045796017000580 (2019).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Lim, M. H., Eres, R. & Vasan, S. Understanding loneliness in the twenty-first century: An update on correlates, risk factors, and potential solutions. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-020-01889-7 (2020).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Victor, C. R. & Yang, K. The prevalence of loneliness among adults: A case study of the United Kingdom. J Psychol 146, 85–104 (2012).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Qualter, P. et al. Loneliness across the lifespan. Pers. Psych. Sci. 10, 250–264. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615568999 (2015).

Article Google Scholar

Spruce, L. Back to basics: social determinants of health. Aorn. J. 110, 60–69. https://doi.org/10.1002/aorn.12722 (2019).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Coughlin, S. S. & Young, L. Social determinants of myocardial infarction risk and survival: A systematic review. Eur. J. Cardiovasc. Res. https://doi.org/10.31487/j.ejcr.2020.01.02 (2020).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Coughlin, S. S. Social determinants of colorectal cancer risk, stage, and survival: A systematic review. Int. J. Colorectal Dis. 35, 985–995. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00384-020-03585-z (2020).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Climie, R. E. et al. Individual and neighborhood deprivation and carotid stiffness. Hypertension 73, 1185–1194. https://doi.org/10.1161/hypertensionaha.118.12186 (2019).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Kim, E. S., Chen, Y., Kawachi, I. & Vander Weele, T. J. Perceived neighborhood social cohesion and subsequent health and well-being in older adults: An outcome-wide longitudinal approach. Health Place 66, 102420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2020.102420 (2020).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Mann, F. et al. A life less lonely: The state of the art in interventions to reduce loneliness in people with mental health problems. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 1, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-017-1392-y (2017).

Article Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T. & Hawkley, L. C. Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 447–454. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005 (2009).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S. & Boomsma, D. I. Evolutionary mechanisms for loneliness. Cogn. Emot. 28, 3–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.837379 (2014).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Lim, M. H., Gleeson, J. F. M., Alvarez-Jimenez, M. & Penn, D. L. Loneliness in psychosis: A systematic review. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 53, 221–238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-018-1482-5 (2018).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Theeke, L. Sociodemographic and health-related risks for loneliness and outcome differences by loneliness status in a sample of US older adults. Res. Gerontol. Nurs. 3, 113–125. https://doi.org/10.3928/19404921-20091103-99 (2010).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Christiansen, J. et al. Loneliness, Social isolation, and chronic disease outcomes. Ann. Behav. Med. https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/kaaa044 (2020).

Article Google Scholar

Surkalim, D. L. et al. The prevalence of loneliness across 113 countries: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 376, e067068. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj-2021-067068 (2022).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Moffitt, T. E., Milne, B. J. & Poulton, R. Socially isolated children 20 years later: risk of cardiovascular disease. Arch. Pediatr. Adoles. Med. 160, 805–811. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.160.8.805 (2006).

Article Google Scholar

Mund, M., Freuding, M. M., Möbius, K., Horn, N. & Neyer, F. J. The stability and change of loneliness across the life span: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 24, 24–52. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868319850738 (2020).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Office for National Statistics. What Characteristics and Circumstances are Associated with Feeling Lonely? https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/articles/lonelinesswhatcharacteristicsandcircumstancesareassociatedwithfeelinglonely/2018-04-10 (2018).

Russell, D. W. UCLA loneliness scale (version 3): Reliability, validity, and factor structure. J. Pers. Assess. 66, 20–40. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa6601_2 (1996).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

De Jong Gierveld, J. & van Tilburg, T. A shortened scale for overall, emotional and social loneliness. Tijdschr. Gerontol. Geriatr. 39, 4–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03078118 (2008).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Martín-María, N. et al. Differential impact of transient and chronic loneliness on health status. A longitudinal study. Psychol. Health 35, 177–195. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2019.1632312 (2020).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Newall, N. E., Chipperfield, J. G. & Bailis, D. S. Predicting stability and change in loneliness in later life. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 31, 335–351. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407513494951 (2014).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Shiovitz-Ezra, S. & Ayalon, L. Situational versus chronic loneliness as risk factors for all-cause mortality. Int. Psychogeriatr. 22, 455–462 (2010).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Zhong, B.-L., Chen, S.-L. & Conwell, Y. Effects of transient versus chronic loneliness on cognitive function in older adults: Findings from the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 24, 389–398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2015.12.009 (2016).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Matthews, T. et al. The developmental course of loneliness in adolescence: Implications for mental health, educational attainment, and psychosocial functioning. Dev. Psychopathol. 1, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579421001632 (2022).

Article Google Scholar

Wilkins, R., Laß, I., Butterworth, P. & Vera-Toscano, E. The Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia Survey: Selected Findings from Waves 1 to 17 (Applied Economic & Social Research, University of Melbourne, 2019).

Google Scholar

Butterworth, P. & Crosier, T. The validity of the SF-36 in an Australian National Household Survey: Demonstrating the applicability of the Household Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) Survey to examination of health inequalities. BMC Public Health 4, 44. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-4-44 (2004).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Lau, S. & Gruen, G. E. The social stigma of loneliness: Effect of target person’s and perceiver’s sex. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 18, 182–189. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167292182009 (1992).

Article Google Scholar

Flood, M. Mapping Loneliness in Australia. The Australia Institute. https://australiainstitute.org.au/wp-content/uploads/2020/12/DP76_8.pdf (2005)

Manera, K. E., Smith, B. J., Owen, K. B., Phongsavan, P. & Lim, M. H. Psychometric assessment of scales for measuring loneliness and social isolation: An analysis of the household, income and labour dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 20, 40. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-022-01946-6 (2022).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

National Centre for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion (NCCDPHP). About Chronic Disease. https://www.cdc.gov/chronicdisease/about/index.htm (2021).

Umberson, D., Lin, Z. & Cha, H. Gender and social isolation across the life course. J. Health Soc. Behav. 63, 319–335. https://doi.org/10.1177/00221465221109634 (2022).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Theeke, L. A. Predictors of loneliness in U.S. adults over age sixty-five. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 23, 387–396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apnu.2008.11.002 (2009).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Tong, S. et al. Geographic characteristics of loneliness in primary care. Ann. Fam. Med. 17, 158–160. https://doi.org/10.1370/afm.2364 (2019).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Lasgaard, M., Friis, K. & Shevlin, M. “Where are all the lonely people?” A population-based study of high-risk groups across the life span. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 51, 1373–1384. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-016-1279-3 (2016).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Lee, S. B., Shin, Y., Jeon, Y. & Kim, S. Factors affecting social isolation among the young adults in South Korea: A cross-sectional analysis. Front. Pub. Health 10, 979136. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2022.979136 (2022).

Article Google Scholar

Becker, C. C. Migrants’ social integration and its relevance for national identification: An empirical comparison across three social spheres. Front. Sociol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fsoc.2021.700580 (2022).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Sawir, E., Marginson, S., Deumert, A., Nyland, C. & Ramia, G. Loneliness and international students: An Australian study. J. Stud. Int. Educ. 12, 148–180. https://doi.org/10.1177/1028315307299699 (2008).

Article Google Scholar

Zhao, I. Y., Holroyd, E., Garrett, N., Wright-StClair, V. A. & Neville, S. Chinese late-life immigrants’ loneliness and social isolation in host countries: An integrative review. J. Clin. Nurs. 32, 1615–1624. https://doi.org/10.1111/jocn.16134 (2023).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Algren, M. H. et al. Social isolation, loneliness, socioeconomic status, and health-risk behaviour in deprived neighbourhoods in Denmark: A cross-sectional study. SSM Popul. Health 10, 100546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2020.100546 (2020).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Australian Bureau of Statistics. Census of Population and Housing: Reflecting Australia: Stories from the Census, 2016. https://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/[email protected]/Lookup/by%20Subject/2071.0~2016~Main%20Features~Socio-Economic%20Advantage%20and%20Disadvantage~123 (2016).

Crowe, C. L. et al. Associations of loneliness and social isolation with health span and life span in the U.S. Health and Retirement study. J. Geront. Ser. 76, 1997–2006. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glab128 (2021).

Article Google Scholar

Summerfield, M. et al. HILDA User Manual-Release 21 (University of Melbourne, Melbourne Institute, 2022).

Google Scholar

Ending Loneliness Together. Ending Loneliness Together in Australia Whitepaper. https://endingloneliness.com.au/wp-content/uploads/2020/11/Ending-Loneliness-Together-in-Australia_Nov20.pdf (2020).

Borys, S. & Perlman, D. Gender differences in loneliness. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 11, 63–74. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167285111006 (1985).

Article Google Scholar

Dahlberg, L., Andersson, L., McKee, K. J. & Lennartsson, C. Predictors of loneliness among older women and men in Sweden: A national longitudinal study. Aging Ment. Health 19, 409–417 (2015).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Pinquart, M. & Sorensen, S. Influences on loneliness in older adults: A meta-analysis. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 23, 245–266. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324834BASP2304_2 (2001).

Article Google Scholar

Pinquart, M. Loneliness in married, widowed, divorced, and never-married older adults. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 20, 31–53 (2003).

Article Google Scholar

Chiu, M. et al. Self-rated health and mental health of lone fathers compared with lone mothers and partnered fathers: A population-based cross-sectional study. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 71, 417–423. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2016-208005 (2017).

Article Google Scholar

Chiu, M. et al. Mortality in single fathers compared with single mothers and partnered parents: A population-based cohort study. Lancet Public. Health 3, e115–e123. https://doi.org/10.1016/s2468-2667(18)30003-3 (2018).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Chen, W., Wu, S., Ling, L. & Renzaho, A. M. N. Impacts of social integration and loneliness on mental health of humanitarian migrants in Australia: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Aust. N. Z. J. Public Health 43, 46–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/1753-6405.12856 (2019).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Creed, P. A. & Reynolds, J. Economic deprivation, experiential deprivation and social loneliness in unemployed and employed youth. J. Commun. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 11, 167–178. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.612 (2001).

Article Google Scholar

Kearns, A., Whitley, E., Tannahill, C. & Ellaway, A. Loneliness, social relations and health and well-being in deprived communities. Psychol. Health Med. 20, 332–344. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2014.940354 (2014).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

McCain, D., Morgan, A. A. & Wright, R. Associations between neighborhood SES disadvantage and feelings of depression and loneliness in older adults. Innov. Aging 5, 917–917. https://doi.org/10.1093/geroni/igab046.3296 (2021).

Article PubMed Central Google Scholar

Astell-Burt, T. et al. More green, less lonely? A longitudinal cohort study. Int. J. Epidemiol. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyab089 (2021).

Article PubMed Central Google Scholar

Christiansen, J. et al. Associations of loneliness and social isolation with physical and mental health among adolescents and young adults. Pers. Public. Health 141, 226–236. https://doi.org/10.1177/17579139211016077 (2021).

Article CAS Google Scholar

Fernández-Alonso, A. M., Trabalón-Pastor, M., Vara, C., Chedraui, P. & Pérez-López, F. R. Life satisfaction, loneliness and related factors during female midlife. Maturitas 72, 88–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.maturitas.2012.02.001 (2012).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Shankar, A., McMunn, A., Banks, J. & Steptoe, A. Loneliness, social isolation, and behavioral and biological health indicators in older adults. Health Psychol. 30, 377 (2011).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Schrempft, S., Jackowska, M., Hamer, M. & Steptoe, A. Associations between social isolation, loneliness, and objective physical activity in older men and women. BMC Public Health 19, 74. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-6424-y (2019).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Mutz, J., Roscoe, C. J. & Lewis, C. M. Exploring health in the UK Biobank: Associations with sociodemographic characteristics, psychosocial factors, lifestyle and environmental exposures. BMC Med. 19, 240. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-021-02097-z (2021).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Qualter, P. et al. Tackling Loneliness Evidence Review: Main Report. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/tackling-loneliness-evidence-review/tackling-loneliness-evidence-review-full-report (2022).

Stickley, A. & Koyanagi, A. Physical multimorbidity and loneliness: a population-based study. PLoS ONE 13, e0191651. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191651 (2018).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Beller, J. & Wagner, A. Disentangling loneliness: Differential effects of subjective loneliness, network quality, network size, and living alone on physical, mental, and cognitive health. J. Aging Health https://doi.org/10.1177/0898264316685843 (2017).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Stansfield, J., South, J. & Mapplethorpe, T. What are the elements of a whole system approach to community-centred public health? A qualitative study with public health leaders in England’s local authority areas. BMJ Open 10, e036044. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2019-036044 (2020).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Download references

Sydney School of Public Health, Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Michelle H. Lim, Karine E. Manera, Katherine B. Owen, Philayrath Phongsavan & Ben J. Smith

Charles Perkins Centre, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Michelle H. Lim, Karine E. Manera, Katherine B. Owen, Philayrath Phongsavan & Ben J. Smith

Iverson Health Innovation Research Institute, Swinburne University of Technology, Victoria, Australia

Michelle H. Lim

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

M.H.L. conceptualized the study and its design and drafted the initial manuscript. K.M. led the statistical analyses, wrote the results section of the manuscript, and contributed to the writing. K.O. contributed to the statistical analyses and contributed to the writing. P.P. and B.S. contributed the conceptualization of the study and its design and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the manuscript.

Correspondence to Michelle H. Lim.

Michelle H. Lim and Ben J. Smith are part of the Scientific Advisory Committee of Ending Loneliness Together. Michelle H. Lim is part of the International Scientific Board of the Global Initiative on Loneliness and Connection.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and Permissions

Lim, M.H., Manera, K.E., Owen, K.B. et al. The prevalence of chronic and episodic loneliness and social isolation from a longitudinal survey. Sci Rep 13, 12453 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-39289-x

Download citation

Received: 29 April 2022

Accepted: 22 July 2023

Published: 01 August 2023

DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-39289-x

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.